Nicotine stimulated increases in c-fos mRNA in the hippocampus, hyperstriatum accessorium, hyperstriatum ventrale, nucleus accumbens, bulbus olfactorius, paleostriatum augmentatum, and stratum griseum et fibrosum superficiale.
The involvement of tenascin-C in this juvenile learning task was tested by injections of monoclonal antibodies directed to distinct domains of the tenascin-C molecule into the avian prefrontal cortex analog, the medio-rostral nidopallium/mesopallium (formerly termed medio-rostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale), a forebrain area which has been shown to be critically involved in auditory filial imprinting.
The intermediate and medial hyperstriatum ventrale/mesopallium (IMHV/IMM), a forebrain memory storage site, was removed together with a control brain region, the posterior pole of the neostriatum/nidopallium (PPN).
In addition, we demonstrated potential for neuronal synthesis of pregnenolone in the day-old chick brain, including in the intermediate medial mesopallium, formerly known as intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale and the medial striatum..
Exposure to SM elicited significant deficits in the intermedial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV)-related imprinting behavior.
We have previously shown that activation of beta2-, beta3- and alpha1-adrenoceptors (ARs) by injection into the multimodal forebrain association region (intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale [ IMHV] or intermediate medial mesopallium [ IMM]) is involved in the consolidation of memory 30 min after training and that activation of alpha2-ARs in the caudate putamen plays a role in the reinforcement of memory leading to consolidation in the IMM (IMHV).
We demonstrate that this transcript is present in a number of forebrain structures including the medio-rostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale (MNH), a region that has been strongly implicated in auditory imprinting (which is a form of recognition memory), and Field L, the avian analog of the mammalian auditory cortex.
Because mammalian models possess inherent confounding factors from maternal effects, we conducted parallel studies using avian embryos, evaluating hyperstriatal nucleus (intermedial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale, IMHV)-related, filial imprinting behavior.
We utilized the developing chick to overcome confounds related to maternal effects and compared the actions of nicotine, chlorpyrifos, and heroin on cholinergic signaling in the intermedial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV), which controls imprinting behavior.
In the present study, we focussed on the medio-rostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale (MNH) of the chick forebrain.
Post-synaptic GABA(B) responses (slow, late hyperpolarisations which can be eliminated by perfusion with phaclofen) can be recorded in vitro from many, but not all, neurones in the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV).
Statistical analyses of regional contributions to the observed impairment identified the left visual wulst and bilateral hyperstriatum ventrale, which lies outside the wulst, as interesting areas.
The intermediate and medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) is an area of the domestic chick forebrain that stores information acquired through the learning process of imprinting.
2-[ (14)C]-deoxyglucose uptake increased in two brain regions, the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) and lobus parolfactorius (LPO) following reminder as it did following training, but the increase was bilateral rather than confined to the left hemisphere and was more marked in LPO than IMHV.
Brain regions [ including the caudomedial part of the neostriatum (NCM) and of the hyperstriatum ventrale (CMHV)] outside the song system show increased neuronal activation, measured as expression of immediate early genes (IEGs), when zebra finch males are exposed to song.
The sectors of the hyperstriatum composing the Wulst (i.e., the hyperstriatum accessorium intermedium, and dorsale), the hyperstriatum ventrale, the neostriatum, and the archistriatum have been renamed (respectively) the hyperpallium (hypertrophied pallium), the mesopallium (middle pallium), the nidopallium (nest pallium), and the arcopallium (arched pallium).
In both birds and primates, innovation rate is positively correlated with the relative size of association areas in the brain, the hyperstriatum ventrale and neostriatum in birds and the isocortex and striatum in primates.
Injection of monosodium glutamate (40nmol/hemisphere) into the intermediate hyperstriatum ventrale of the day-old chick inhibits the formation of short-term memory for a single trial learning that discriminates between colours of beads.
The intermediate and medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) is a part of the chick forebrain that is crucial for imprinting.
Using the chick 1-day-old passive avoidance learning paradigm and differential display RNA fingerprinting, we identified 13 candidate genes which are upregulated in the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV), an area that has been correlated with the initial processing of memory formation.
HVC (nidopallial area, formerly known as hyperstriatum ventrale pars caudalis), a key centre for song control in oscines, responds in a selective manner to conspecific songs as indicated by electrophysiology.
We show that the entopallium projects laterally and diffusely to the perientopallium and nidopallium (formerly the neostriatum) and specifically and densely to a teardrop-shaped nucleus in the ventrolateral mesopallium (formerly known as the hyperstriatum ventrale), here called MVL (abbreviation used as a proper name).
In vivo microdialysis and behavioural studies in the domestic chick have shown that glutamatergic as well as monoaminergic neurotransmission in the medio-rostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale (MNH) is altered after auditory filial imprinting.
The caudal hyperstriatum ventrale (cHV) in both sexes also responds to song, but in females not as selectively as the NCM and HP.
Birds hearing hatch dialect showed greater ZENK induction in the caudomedial hyperstriatum ventrale and the dorsal portion of the caudomedial neostriatum than birds hearing foreign dialect, supporting previous work showing a relationship between ZENK and salience of the stimulus. In the dorsal portion of the caudomedial neostriatum, ZENK induction was correlated with the amount of non-vocal courtship behavior; however, in the caudomedial hyperstriatum ventrale, ZENK induction was more highly correlated with the females' own vocal behavior and thus may have been partly self-induced.
The memory enhancing action of noradrenaline injected into the basal ganglia (lobus parolfactorius-LPO) was reduced in the presence of the alpha(2)-adrenoceptor antagonist yohimbine, but when noradrenaline was injected into the multi-modal association area (intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale-IMHV), yohimbine failed to prevent memory enhancement.
The archistriatum mediates a neural pathway from the medial part of intermediate hyperstriatum ventrale (in the dorsal pallium) to the lobus parolfactorius (in the medial striatum), thus is possibly involved in memory formation in the domestic chick.
The projections of the DMA covered the rostrobasal forebrain, ventral paleostriatum, nucleus accumbens, septal nuclei, Wulst, hyperstriatum ventrale, neostriatal areas, archistriatal subdivisions, dorsolateral corticoid area, numerous hypothalamic nuclei, and dorsal thalamic nuclei.
Previous work has identified the intermediate and medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) as a region of the chick brain storing information acquired through the learning process of imprinting.
Expression was highest in the caudomedial neostriatum (NCM), lower in the caudomedial hyperstriatum ventrale (CMHV), and lowest in the hippocampus.
We then examined their IEG response to novel long-bout versus novel short-bout songs by quantifying ZENK protein in two song-processing areas: the caudo-medial hyperstriatum ventrale and the caudo-medial neostriatum.
Furthermore, the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin (30 mM; 10 microl per hemisphere) injected into the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale 15 min pre-training or 45 min post-training blocked long-term memory for the DTAL task when tested 24 h later.
Unbiased stereological analysis of labelled cells in selected forebrain areas 24 h post BrdU injection showed a significant MeA-training induced increase in labelled cells in both the dorsal VZ surface bordering the intermediate and medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) and the tuberculum olfactorium (TO).
A model is developed that integrates noradrenergic activity in basal ganglia (lobus parolfactorius (LPO)) and association cortex (intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV)) leading to the consolidation of memory 30 min after training.
medio-rostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale region (MNH), is involved in juvenile auditory filial imprinting.
In the few avian species investigated, areas outside of the circuit for song production contain neurons that are active following song presentation, specifically the caudal hyperstriatum ventrale (cHV) and caudomedial neostriatum (NCM).
The mediorostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale and neostriatum dorsocaudale of the domestic chick are crucially involved in filial imprinting and are major targets of mesotelencephalic dopaminergic projections. To better understand the functional role of dopamine in these forebrain regions, the ultrastructure of dopamine terminals was studied by serial section electron microscopy using immunohistochemical labeling with antibodies to tyrosine hydroxylase and dopamine.At light as well as electron microscopic level, dopamine and tyrosine hydroxylase-immunoreactive fibers were present at moderate densities in the mediorostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale and high densities in the neostriatum dorsocaudale. The frequency of tyrosine hydroxylase-immunoreactive profiles per unit area was significantly higher in the neostriatum dorsocaudale than in the mediorostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale. In both regions, tyrosine hydroxylase-immunoreactive terminals were relatively small, with mean areas of 0.55 microm(2) in the mediorostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale and 0.48 microm(2) in the neostriatum dorsocaudale. The majority of tyrosine hydroxylase-immunoreactive synapses were symmetrical (83% in the mediorostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale, 75% in the neostriatum dorsocaudale) as opposed to asymmetrical (17 and 25%, respectively), but there were also tyrosine hydroxylase-immunoreactive terminals which lacked clear synaptic specializations. The preferred targets of the synaptic tyrosine hydroxylase-immunoreactive terminals were dendritic shafts (64% in the mediorostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale, 63% in the neostriatum dorsocaudale) and less frequently dendritic spines (17 and 23%, respectively) or perikarya (19 and 14%, respectively).
Cocultures of the learning-relevant forebrain region mediorostral neostriatum and hyperstriatum ventrale (MNH) and its main glutamatergic input area nucleus dorsomedialis anterior thalami/posterior thalami were morphologically and physiologically characterized.
Two of the forebrain areas highly active during first courtship (as demonstrated by the 14C-2-deoxyglucose technique), the imprinting locus latral neo/hyperstriatum ventrale (LNH) and the secondary visual area hyperstriatum accessorium/dorsale (HAD), demonstrate enhanced fos expression. Two other imprinting-related areas, the medial neo/hyperstriatum ventrale (MNH) and archistriatum/neostriatum caudale (ANC), do show c-fos induction; however, the areas are not congruous with those demarcated by the 2-DG autoradiographic studies.
Auditory nuclei included the Field L complex (L1, L2a, and L3), the neostriatum intermedium pars ventrolateralis (NIVL), the neostriatum adjacent to caudal nucleus basalis (peri-basalis or pBas), an area in the frontal lateral neostriatum (NFl), the supracentral nucleus of the lateral neostriatum (NLs), and the ventromedial hyperstriatum ventrale (HVvm). The results indicated that tracer injections in the vocal nucleus HVo (oval nucleus of the hyperstriatum ventrale), in fields lateral to HVo and in NLs labeled many Zenk-positive neurons in HVvm, NFl, and NLs.
Likewise, isochronic clusters of the hyperstriatum ventrale relate to patchy heterogeneities in the cadherin-7 immunoreactivity pattern.
We have further examined this by following the time course of glutamate and gamma-amino butyric acid release from slices prepared from the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale of day-old chicks (Ross 1 Chunky) trained to avoid a bead covered in the aversant methylanthranilate. At various times after training, slices of left and right intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale were incubated in medium containing 50 mM potassium chloride and amino acid release was determined. In the right intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale of methylanthranilate birds glutamate release was increased from 3 to 6.5 h and gamma-amino butyric acid at 6.5 h: a time that corresponded to the mobilization of a late process required if long-term memory was to be formed. These results confirm that the amino acids glutamate and gamma-amino butyric acid are released from the intermediate hyperstriatum ventrale in a calcium-dependent, neurotransmitter-like manner.
Radioimmunoassay examination of the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale 5 or 30 min after training or the lobus parolfactorius 60 or 120 min after training on the passive avoidance task did not show learning-related differences in absolute levels of DHEA or DHEA-S.
Activation of alpha1-adrenoceptors in the intermediate hyperstriatum ventrale in the chick forebrain by the alpha1 adrenoceptor agonist methoxamine inhibits the consolidation of memory.
The intermediate and medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) is a forebrain region in the domestic chick that is a site of information storage for the learning process of imprinting.
Immediate early gene expression was also increased in telencephalic areas such as the hyperstriatum ventrale that presumably plays a role in the integration of sensory cues related to female recognition.
Recent evidence showed that exposure of tape-tutored zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata castanotis) males to the tutor song involves neuronal activation in brain regions outside the conventional 'song control pathways', particularly the caudal part of the neostriatum (NCM) and of the hyperstriatum ventrale (CMHV).
The intermediate, medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) is an area of the forebrain of the domestic chick which exhibits great plasticity.
Training chicks (Gallus domesticus) on a one-trial passive avoidance task results in transient and time-dependent enhanced increases in N-methyl-d-aspartate- or alpha-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionate-stimulated intracellular calcium concentration in synaptoneurosomes isolated from a specific forebrain region, the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale. Preincubation of synaptoneurosomes, isolated from the intermediate hyperstriatum ventrale 10 min after training, with 100 nM dantrolene abolished the enhanced training-induced increase in intracellular calcium concentration elicited by 0.5 mM N-methyl-d-aspartate.
There is strong converging evidence that the intermediate and medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale of the chick brain is a memory store for information acquired through the learning process of imprinting. Therefore, the responses of the intermediate and medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale neurons to a visual imprinting stimulus were determined before, during, and after training.
The afferent and efferent projections of a vocal control nucleus, the oval nucleus of the hyperstriatum ventrale (HVo), were mapped out in a parrot, the budgerigar (Melopsittacus undulatus) to determine the relationships of this nucleus to the auditory system.
Using multiple regressions on 17 avian taxa, we show that an operational estimate of behavioral flexibility, the frequency of feeding innovation reports in ornithology journals, is most closely predicted by relative size of one of these DVR areas, the hyperstriatum ventrale. Similar results are found when the hyperstriatum ventrale is lumped with a second DVR structure, the neostriatum. Our results parallel those on primates showing a correlation between innovation rate and neocortex size and support the idea that the mammalian neocortex and the neostriatum-hyperstriatum ventrale complex in birds have similar integrative roles..
The intermediate and medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) of the chick brain is a site of recognition memory for filial imprinting.
D1B receptor mRNA was abundant in the medial and lateral striatum, and in the pallial region termed the hyperstriatum ventrale, and moderately abundant in the intralaminar dorsal and posterior thalamus and in the superficial gray of the optic tectum. The data suggest that while both D1A and D1B receptors mediate dopaminergic responses in many neurons of the avian striatum, primarily D1A receptors mediate dopaminergic responses in the archistriatum and the neostriatum, while primarily D1B receptors mediate dopaminergic responses in the hyperstriatum ventrale and the thalamus..
Extracellular recording techniques were used to study the effects of the nitric oxide releasing agents diethylamine-NO (DEA-NO) and S-nitroso-N-acetyl-penicillamine (SNAP) on synaptic transmission in the intermediate and medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV), a part of the domestic chick forebrain that is essential for some forms of early learning.
Areas with densely packed GRP-ir clusters of varicosities were the medial intermediate hyperstriatum ventrale and lateral septal nucleus; dense GRP-ir neuropil was found in the parolfactory lobe, and in the dorsal half of the intermediate and caudal archistriatum.
Training chicks on a one trial passive avoidance task results in a molecular cascade in a specific brain region, the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale.
A restricted part of the intermediate and medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) of the domestic chick forebrain is pivotal to the learning process of imprinting and is probably the site at which information about an imprinting stimulus is stored.
Memory formation for a passive avoidance task in the domestic chick is likely to involve a hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV)-archistriatum-lobus parolfactorius (LPO) arc.
Recent evidence suggests that song perception in songbirds involves neuronal activation in brain regions that have not traditionally been implicated in the control of song production or song learning, notably the caudal part of the neostriatum (NCM) and of the hyperstriatum ventrale. When, as adults, they were reexposed to the tutor song, the males showed increased expression of the protein products of the immediate early genes egr-1 (ZENK) and c-fos in the NCM and caudal hyperstriatum ventrale, but not in the conventional "song-control nuclei." The strength of the immediate early gene response (which is a reflection of neuronal activation) in the NCM correlated significantly and positively with the number of song elements that the birds had copied from the tutor song.
By contrast, in the appetitive sexual behavior group, significant increases in deoxyglucose incorporation were observed in two telencephalic areas, the intermediate hyperstriatum ventrale and neostriatum caudolaterale by comparison with the controls, but decreases were detected in the stratum griseum et fibrosum superficiale of optic tectum by comparison with the consummatory behavior group.
The present work describes interactions between adenosine and the amino acids glutamate and GABA in slices of intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV), an area of the chick brain known to be involved in learning and memory events associated with a one-trial passive avoidance task.
Localization of the ZENK protein product revealed that song stimulation resulted in a significant increase in the number of ZENK immunoreactive (-ir) cells in the caudomedial neostriatum (NCM) and the caudomedial hyperstriatum ventrale (cmHV) compared with unstimulated birds.
Sexual interactions significantly induced FLI cells in the hyperstriatum ventrale, the part of the archistriatum just lateral to the anterior commissure, and the nucleus intercollicularis.
Two hours post-training there was an increase in the number of Fos-positive stained nuclei in right intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) (P < 0.01), left IMHV (P < 0.05), right lobus parolfactorius (LPO) (P < 0.025) and left LPO (P < 0.05) of birds trained on the bitter bead compared with controls that had pecked a water-coated bead.
Recordings were made in the intermediate and medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale of behaving domestic chicks which had been imprinted (trained) by being exposed to a training stimulus.
Previous work has shown that, after domestic chicks have learned the characteristics of an object (visual imprinting), there is a learning-related increase in the numerical density of Fos-immunopositive neurons in the intermediate and medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale, a forebrain region that is a site of recognition memory for the imprinted object. Sections from the chicks' brains were stained for Fos-like immunoreactivity, and the numerical density of Fos-positive nuclei in the intermediate and medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale was counted. Relative to untrained chicks, there was a 60% increase in the number of Fos-positive nuclei in the intermediate and medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale 2 h after the start of training (P = 0.02), but not at any other time. Approximately 95% of the Fos-positive neurons in the intermediate and medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale were also immunopositive for GABA. In neurons immunopositive for GABA, there were significantly (P = 0.02) more Fos-positive nuclei in the intermediate and medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale 2 h after the start of training than in untrained chicks.
Among these, within minutes of training, there is a transient, enhanced release of glutamate and increased agonist and antagonist binding to N-methyl-D-aspartate-sensitive glutamate receptors in the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale of the forebrain. To test this possibility, we have measured the calcium concentration in synaptoneurosomes, containing both pre- and postsynaptic elements, prepared from left and right intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale at various times following training, using Fura 2-AM as the indicator of intracellular calcium concentration. N-Methyl-D-aspartate (0.5 mM) induced a significant enhancement in the increase in calcium concentration in intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale from both left and right hemispheres 10 min and 30 min after training.
Highly arborized terminal fields were found all along this pathway, notably in the medial parolfactory lobe (corresponding to the basal ganglia) and along the dorsal roof of the rostral hyperstriatum ventrale just ventral to the laminal frontalis superior (in ring doves) and the lamina frontalis suprema (in starlings).
A significant increase occurred bilaterally in the quantity of bound alpha-BgT in the lobus parolfactorius, while the amount of bound QNB decreased significantly, and bilaterally, in the hippocampus, hyperstriatum ventrale, lobus parolfactorius and posterolateral telencephalon, pars dorsalis.
The stimulation of adenylyl cyclase (AC) by dopamine was investigated in membrane fractions of the forebrain areas mediorostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale (MNH) and lobus parolfactorius (LPO) of 8-day-old domestic chicks that had been raised under different social conditions: group A, socially isolated; group B, imprinted on an acoustic stimulus; group C, trained but nonimprinted; and group D, reared in small groups.
Lesion studies show that the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV), a forebrain visual association area in chicks, is involved in learning and memory for one-trial passive avoidance and imprinting.
The associative avian forebrain region medio-rostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale (MNH) is involved in auditory filial imprinting and may be considered the avian analogue of the mammalian prefrontal cortex.
The nuclei investigated were the hypoglossal nucleus, dorsomedial nucleus of the intercollicular midbrain, central nucleus of the archistriatum, central nucleus of the lateral neostriatum, oval nucleus of the hyperstriatum ventrale, medial division of the oval nucleus of the anterior neostriatum, and magnocellular nucleus of the lobus parolfactorius.
The lateral pallium comprises the hyperstriatum ventrale, overlying temporo-parieto-occipital corticoid layer and piriform cortex, plus dorsal intermediate and posterior archistriatum.
Intense gastrin releasing peptide (GRP)-immunoreactivity was found in the neuropil of LPO, the ventral paleostriatum and the caudal archistriatum; further GRP-immunoreactive varicosities were found in the neostriatum and the hyperstriatum ventrale - particularly in its medial part - whereas GRP-immunoreactive cells occurred in the medial neostriatum, the hyperstriatum accessorium and the ventral archistriatum.
Intracerebral injections of 5 microl of antibodies to brain-derived neurotrophic factor into the left and right intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale resulted in a dose-dependent reduction in avoidance of an "aversive" bead by 3 h after training. In lysed synaptosomal membranes prepared from chicks injected with antibodies to brain-derived neurotrophic factor there was a decrease in expression of SNAP-25 and syntaxin in the left, but not the right, intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale, a region known to be associated with memory formation, which correlated with the decrease in neurotrophin concentration.
The basal phosphorylation of CaMKII from the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) and lobus parolfactorius (LPO) regions of the chick brain is shown to be largely right hemisphere-lateralized.
Auditory filial imprinting in the domestic chicken is accompanied by a dramatic loss of spine synapses in two higher associative forebrain areas, the mediorostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale (MNH) and the dorsocaudal neostriatum (Ndc).
Using the classified chicks, the acetylcholine (ACh) and glutamate releases from the medial hyperstriatum ventrale (MHV) of the chick forebrains were determined by in vivo microdialysis.
In the young chick, the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale is involved in learning paradigms, including imprinting and passive avoidance learning. Biochemical changes in the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale following learning include an up-regulation of amino-acid transmitter levels and receptor activity. The results suggest that such changes in extracellular glutamate levels in the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale accompany pecking at either the water- or the methylanthranylate-bead.
Auditory filial imprinting induces quantitative changes of synaptic density in the forebrain area mediorostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale of the domestic chick.
Besides other projections, the visual Wulst emitted fibres also to the middle and lateral parts of the hyperstriatum ventrale. Further experiments revealed that the middle part of the hyperstriatum ventrale projected to the ectostriatum centrale and periphericum and established an indirect connection between the visual Wulst and the ectostriatum. The lateral part of the hyperstriatum ventrale sent a few efferent fibres toward the diencephalon and brainstem, but projected massively to the ectostriatum periphericum, neostriatum intermedium pars laterale, the ventral part of the neostriatum caudale and the archistriatum dorsale.
However, following lesion to cHV (caudal hyperstriatum ventrale, an auditory area) females performed courtship displays at high rates in response to both conspecific and heterospecific song.
The hippocampus of the chick projects to the intermediate and medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) which stores information acquired through the learning process of imprinting.
The learning process of imprinting involves morphological, electrophysiological and biochemical changes in a region of the chick (Gallus gallus domesticus) forebrain known as the intermediate and medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV). In this context, it has previously been reported that a novel avian gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) type A (GABA(A)) receptor gene, encoding the gamma4 subunit, is highly expressed in the hyperstriatum ventrale. At 10 h but not at 5 h after training there is a decrease (25-32%) in the amount of this transcript in parts of the medial hyperstriatum ventrale, including the IMHV.
In behaving, imprinted chicks with no experience of objects at different distances, neuronal activity was recorded from the intermediate and medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV), a brain region crucial for the recognition memory underlying imprinting.
At day 20, the hyperstriatum ventrale, pars caudale is the only song nucleus in which neurons showed BDNF immunoreactivity. At day 45, the staining in hyperstriatum ventrale, pars caudale was denser than at day 20 and the robust nucleus of the archistriatum, another song nucleus, showed BDNF labeling.
Part of the hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) serves such a function for the learning process of imprinting in domestic chicks.
The intermediate and medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) of the chick forebrain is a site of recognition memory for the learning process of imprinting.
Bilateral pharmacological manipulation of the intermediate hyperstriatum ventrale was performed by intracerebral application of an alpha2-noradrenergic agonist, clonidine (5 microM), or an antagonist, rauwolscine (300 microM). Scatchard analysis revealed that MeA-training resulted in a significant bilateral upregulation in the number of [ 3H]rauwolscine binding sites (Bmax) in the area of hyperstriatum ventrale.
Special emphasis was put on the connections of the dNC complex with other imprinting relevant regions in the rostral telencephalon, such as the mediorostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale (MNH) and the intermediate and medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV).
Birds that heard the playbacks and did not sing in response showed increased ZENK protein levels in auditory brain areas, including the caudomedial neostriatum and hyperstriatum ventrale, fields L1 and L3, the shelf adjacent to the high vocal center (HVC), the cup adjacent to the nucleus robustus archistriatalis (RA), and the nucleus mesencephalicus lateralis pars dorsalis (MLd).
In the hyperstriatum ventrale, a chick forebrain area involved in the passive avoidance task.
In this study, we show that the amount of cell proliferation in the ventricular zone of the hippocampus (HP) and the hyperstriatum ventrale (HV) is influenced by behavioural experience.
In the intermediate and medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV), a telencephalic region essentially involved in the initial processes of early learning tasks in poultry chicks, induction of an immediate early gene c-fos correlates significantly with the degree of learning (K.V.
One such area is the hyperstriatum ventrale (HV).
Long-term memory for a passive avoidance task in day-old chicks has proved to depend upon an action of the adrenal steroid corticosterone through specific receptors in a brain region, the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV), involved in learning the task.
Particular attention was paid to two forebrain regions, the mediorostral neostriatum/ hyperstriatum ventrale (MNH) and neostriatum dorsocaudale (Ndc), which have been shown to be crucially involved in filial imprinting.
Strong converging evidence indicates that the intermediate and medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) of the chick forebrain is a site of recognition memory for the learning process of imprinting.
However, pharmacological blockade of NMDA-receptors in the dorso-caudal neostriatum leads to a marked suppression of stimulus-evoked 2-FDG uptake in the dorso-caudal neostriatum and also in the interconnected imprinting relevant forebrain area, medio-rostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale (MNH).
Copulation induced the appearance of Fos-like immunoreactive (FLI) cells in the preoptic area, the hyperstriatum ventrale, parts of the archistriatum, and the nucleus intercollicularis.
The anatomical circuitry underlying memory formation in the chick is likely to involve the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale-archistriatum-LPO arc.
In the forebrain of the domestic chick (Gallus gallus domesticus), an area termed the mediorostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale is strongly involved in emotional learning paradigms such as acoustic filial imprinting. Furthermore, the involvement of the mediorostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale in stressful situations, such as social separation, has been demonstrated in 2-deoxyglucose studies. A small, but statistically significant, decrease of extracellular homovanillic acid levels was found in the mediorostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale of imprinted chicks compared to control animals, whereas changes of 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid were not detected. In a second experiment, we investigated the levels of homovanillic acid and 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid in the mediorostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale of socially reared chicks during different stress situations, such as handling or separation from their cage mates. These results provide the first evidence that the physiological response of the mediorostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale related to different emotional conditions after acoustic imprinting and during stressful situations is, at least in part, mediated by dopaminergic and/or serotonergic pathways.
This study examined effects on memory formation produced by [ Leu]enkephalin and [ Met]enkephalin administration in 2 regions of the 2-day-old chick brain involved in memory formation: the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) and the lobus parolfactorius (LPO).
In in vivo electrophysiological studies, we analysed whether imprinting alters the responsiveness of neurons to acoustic imprinting stimuli in the mediorostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale. We compared the response characteristics of neurons in the mediorostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale in freely behaving or anesthetized acoustically imprinted, non-imprinted (naive controls) and passive control chicks (stimulus-exposed) during presentation of either the imprinting stimulus or an unfamiliar discrimination stimulus. In acoustically imprinted chicks, the multiunit activity in anesthetized chicks and the fast Fourier transform power spectrum in freely behaving chicks in the mediorostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale were significantly changed during playback of the learned stimulus in comparison to spontaneous activity and compared to the activity during playback of the unfamiliar discrimination stimulus. These results indicate that neurons in the mediorostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale change their responsiveness towards learned, behaviorally relevant stimuli during auditory filial imprinting..
A detailed analysis of the binding capacity for [ 125I]CLO was performed for parts of several functional systems: hypothalamic structures (nucleus inferior hypothalami, nucleus magnocellularis preopticus, nucleus paraventricularis), limbic system (habenula, nucleus septalis lateralis, nucleus striae terminalis), circumventricular organs (organum pineale, organum subfornicale, plexus choroidei ventriculi tertii and ventriculi lateralis), visual system (hyperstriatum accessorium, nucleus reticularis superior, tectum opticum), and associative cortex (hyperstriatum ventrale).
The mediorostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale (MNH) and neostriatum dorsocaudale (Ndc) of the domestic chick are crucially involved in auditory filial imprinting, whereas the lobus parolfactorius (LPO) seems to be involved in the emotional modulation of behavior.
Auditory neurons in the forebrain nucleus HVc (hyperstriatum ventrale pars caudale) are highly sensitive to the temporal structure of the bird's own song.
Bilateral, or unilateral, intracranial injections of 5 microliters of 600 mumol.l-1 D-cycloserine (DCS) were made into the intermediate and medial hyperstriatum ventrale 5 min posttraining and chicks were tested either 30 min or 1, 6, or 24 h later.
The left and right intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) in the chick forebrain have previously been implicated in the formation of memory for this task.
NCL receives telencephalic projections from the hyperstriatum accessorium, cells along the border of hyperstriatum dorsale and hyperstriatum ventrale, anterolateral hyperstriatum adjacent to the vallecula, confined cell groups within the anterior neostriatum, and subdivisions of the archistriatum.
The intermediate, medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) is an area of the avian forebrain which is essential for two forms of early learning in the domestic chick.
Day-old chicks trained on a single trial passive avoidance learning task showed a significant increase, relative to untrained controls, in activity of the Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase (CaMK) in the particulate fraction from tissues from the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale region of the forebrain.
Two nuclei, the oval nucleus of the hyperstriatum ventrale (HVo) and the oval nucleus of the anterior neostriatum (NAo), contained no ChAT labeled somata, dense ChAT labeled fibers and varicosities, and moderate to high levels of AChE staining.
The rostro-medial neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale (MNH) and the neostriatum dorsocaudale (Ndc) are forebrain regions which play a role in auditory filial imprinting.
In particular, no evidence exists of direct connections between auditory and vocal motor pathways, and two newly identified centers for auditory processing, caudomedial neostriatum (Ncm) and caudomedial hyperstriatum ventrale (cmHV), have no documented place among known auditory circuits.
Injection of drugs directly into the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) of day-old chicks, prior to training on a chrome bead dipped in either the strong aversant methyl anthranilate (MeA), or the weak aversant quinine, allows investigation of the effects of potential amnestic and memory-enhancing agents on retention of a passive avoidance task.
Newly hatched domestic chicks were injected uni- or bilaterally into the imprinting relevant forebrain region mediostral neostriatum/ hyperstriatum ventrale (MNH) with different concentrations (1, 3, 12.5, 50 nmol) of the competitive NMDA antagonist DL-5-amino-5- phosphonovaleric acid (APV) prior to exposure to the imprinting stimulus (400-Hz tone pulses).
The forebrain area mediorostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale (MNH) is a center for acoustic imprinting.
Brain slices were prepared from both groups, containing the left, intermediate, medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV)--a region essential for this form of early learning.
The forebrain area medio-rostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale, a presumed analogue to the mammalian prefrontal cortex, displays a variety of synaptic changes during auditory filial imprinting. In order to study the underlying basic mechanisms of this synaptic plasticity we developed slice cultures of the medio-rostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale from newly hatched chicks. Since in situ the medio-rostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale has been previously shown to contain three distinct neuron populations characterized by the activity-regulated Ca(2+)-binding proteins parvalbumin, calbindin D28K and calretinin, we used these proteins as neuronal markers to study the survival and preservation of the morphological features of medio-rostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale neurons in vitro. The immunoreactive neurons in medio-rostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale cultures to a certain extent appear to form synaptic contacts with each other, shown by the double immuncytochemical experiments. This and our previous study on neuronal morphology demonstrates that morphologically and biochemically intact neurons can be maintained in medio-rostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale slice cultures, which may thus provide a suitable in vitro system for further studies of neuronal and synaptic plasticity in vitro..
High levels of specific binding of the D1 and D2 ligands were found in the striatal regions (paleostriatum augmentatum and lobus parolfactorius) of the one-day-old chick, as reported previously in the pigeon, turtle and rat, whilst binding levels were considerably lower in the pallidum (paleostriatum primitivum), hippocampus and hyperstriatum ventrale.
The phosphorylation of MARCKS, but not protein F1/GAP-43, is increased in the intermediate and medial portion of the hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) after chick imprinting.
The intermediate, medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) is a region of the avian forebrain which is known to be essential for early learning in the domestic chick.
The amnesic effects of protein kinase inhibitors (H-7, HA-156, TFP, W-9, and W-13) on memory formation for a one trial peck-avoidance task in chicks were investigated with bilateral and unilateral injections into either the left or the right intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) or the left or right lobus parolfactorius (LPO).
The first memory is formed rapidly and is located in the intermediate and medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) of the left hemisphere; the formation of the second, in another memory system, S', takes several hours and can be prevented by a lesion placed in the right IMHV soon after training.
The role of the noradrenergic system as a modulator of neurotransmission in the chick forebrain was investigated in brain slices containing the medial hyperstriatum ventrale, an area known to be involved in learning and memory.
Neurons in the songbird forebrain area HVc (hyperstriatum ventrale pars caudale or high vocal center) are sensitive to the temporal structure of the bird's own song and are capable of integrating auditory information over a period of several hundred milliseconds.
A significant decrease in KD for the low-affinity component occurred bilaterally in the intermediate and medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV; left, 34.8%; right, 33.3%), a region that has previously been shown to be implicated in the processes of memory formation, following passive avoidance training.
The persistence of morphological features of neurons in slice cultures of the imprinting-relevant forebrain area MNH (mediorostral neostriatum and hyperstriatum ventrale) of the domestic chick was analysed at 7, 14, 21 and 28 days in vitro.
Two regions in the forebrain of domestic chicks (Gallus domesticus), the intermediate and medial hyperstriatum ventrale and the lobus parolfactorius, have previously been shown to be important centres of biochemical, pharmacological and physiological change following one-trial passive avoidance training. The purpose of the present study was to examine, at the electron microscopic level, the fine spatial re-arrangement of synaptic structures in the intermediate and medial hyperstriatum ventrale (at 30 min), and in the lobus parolfactorius (at 24 h), post-training using comprehensive biometrical designs, image analysis and stochastic approaches. In intermediate and medial hyperstriatum ventrale, no significant differences in the numerical density of synapses either between control and trained chicks, or between hemispheres, were revealed using the disector method. However, after training, a nested-ANOVA demonstrated an increase in the thickness of pre- and post-synaptic electron densities (estimated via image analysis) only in the left intermediate and medial hyperstriatum ventrale, whereas synaptic apposition zone profiles increased in length bilaterally. In presynaptic terminals from the intermediate and medial hyperstriatum ventrale, stochastic analysis revealed that training resulted in the re-distribution of synaptic vesicles between two spatial pools relative to synaptic apposition zones, in both hemispheres producing a large number of synaptic vesicles closer to synaptic apposition zones; a nearest neighbour analysis of synaptic apposition zone profiles indicated that the lateral shape of the synaptic apposition zone after training is more complex in both hemispheres. These data suggest that the initial acquisition of memory involves population changes in the fine spatial organization of synaptic vesicles and synaptic apposition zones in synapses in the intermediate and medial hyperstriatum ventrale, which indicate a possible tendency towards greater synaptic efficacies.
The phosphorylation of the myristoylated alanine-rich protein kinase C substrate (MARCKS) in the left intermediate and medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) of the chick brain has been shown previously to correlate significantly with the strength of learning in filial imprinting. However, there was PKC alpha beta-staining of some fibres in the IMHV (but little elsewhere in the hyperstriatum ventrale), in the neostriatum, paleostriatal complex and the lobus parolfactorius.
The responsiveness of neurons in a region of the chick brain involved in the learning process of imprinting, the right intermediate and medial hyperstriatum ventrale (right IMHV), has been investigated in unanaesthetized, trained and untrained chicks.
Training chicks on a one-trial passive avoidance task results in transient up-regulation of the N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor in the left intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) of the forebrain 30 min post-training.
Serrano et al., 1994) and the in vitro inhibition of PKC by CHELE: (a) CHELE, injected into the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale, would significantly impair memory formation; (b) the amnestic dose would be approximately 10 nmol; (c) CHELE would not produce amnesia for about 45 min after training, but significantly impair memory by 60 min.
Increases (805 in the left lobus parolfactorius and 67% in the left intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale) in NMDA sensitive [ 3H]L-glutamate binding occurred in electro-shocked chicks which showed recall of the aversive experience but were absent in MeA-trained chicks rendered amnesic by electro-shock.
Whole-cell perforated-patch recording in slices revealed synaptic organization of the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale, a telencephalic region intimately involved in the early learning processes in chicks.
One-trial passive avoidance training in day-old chicks results in a biochemical cascade occurring in two forebrain regions, the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale and the lobus parolfactorius.
Following training, a sequence of biochemical, electro-physiological, pharmacological and morphological events takes place within two loci in the forebrain, the intermediate and medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV), and part of the paleostriatal complex, the lobus parolfactorius (LPO).
A quantitative immunoelectron microscopic study was carried out in the intermediate portion of hyperstriatum ventrale of chick forebrain (a region exhibiting learning-related morphological plasticity) in order to examine the fine distribution of the neural cell adhesion molecule (N-CAM).
Glutamate NMDA-type receptor binding in the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) of dark-hatched chicks is lateralized.
A brain nucleus that is important for the generation of song in the adult male zebra finch (Poephila guttata), the robust nucleus of the archistriatum (RA), receives dual inputs from two other telencephalic song nuclei: the hyperstriatum ventrale pars caudale (HVc) and the lateral magnocellular nucleus of the anterior neostriatum (L-MAN).
The intermediate and medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) in the chick is involved in memory formation following one-trial passive avoidance training.
NOS was sparsely present in the hyperstriatum ventrale, providing evidence against the involvement of nitric oxide in certain forms of learning and memory processes known to occur in this region..
The intermediate and medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) is a part of the chick forebrain that is critical for the learning process of imprinting and may be a site of information storage. No significant effects of training were found in the anterior hyperstriatum ventrale, lobus parolfactorius, neostriatum, medial hippocampal region, or ventrolateral hippocampal region, but counts in this last region were positively correlated with training approach.
These areas include subfields of field L (L1 and L3), the caudomedial neostriatum (NCM), the caudomedial hyperstriatum ventrale (CMHV) anterior to field L, the caudal paleostriatum, and two field L targets, HVC shelf and RA cup.
In contrast, in the hyperstriatum dorsale and hyperstriatum ventrale, numerous PPE mRNA-expressing cells were detected in the chicken but not in the pigeon.
The chick archistriatum receives afferents from the intermediate part of the medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) and projects to the lobus parolfactorius (LPO). Following iontophoresis of Phaseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin into the medial hyperstriatum ventrale (including the IMHV) of 1-week-old domestic chicks, anterogradely labelled fibers were observed to descend through the medial neostriatum and paleostriatum to enter the archistriatum. These medial hyperstriatum ventrale afferents arborised profusely to give varicose axon branches within all except the anterior part of the archistriatum. Electron microscope examination of Phaseolus lectin immunocytochemistry and Golgi impregnation revealed that medial hyperstriatum ventrale axons formed multiple asymmetric synapses with dendritic spines (head and neck regions) on the terminal and preterminal dendritic segments of densely spiny archistriatal projection neurons. Medial hyperstriatum ventrale afferents were not observed to contact calbindin immunoreactive, presumptive "local circuit" neurons, within the archistriatum, despite a spatial overlap in their distribution.
The developmental sequence of metabolic activity levels in the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV), a region involved with imprinting memory formation, was higher on E19 and D1 than on E20.
In urethane-anesthetized Fringilla montifringilla, effects of electrically stimulating the hyperstriatum ventrale pars caudale (HVc) on vocalization and respiration were observed.
However, non-N-methyl-D-aspartate binding sites in forebrain regions such as hippocampus and hyperstriatum ventrale appeared to be different in being equally sensitive to AMPA and kainate..
The intermediate and medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) of the chick brain is known to be essential for the learning process of imprinting.
The tactile/trigeminal circuit consists of a pattern of reciprocal connections of the nucleus basalis with the overlying parts of the frontal neostriatum (Nf) and hyperstriatum ventrale (HV).
The effects of pre-hatch light exposure on synaptic development in the intermediate and medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) of the chick brain were investigated.
Bilateral intracerebral injections of the PLA2 and lipoxygenase inhibitor nordihydroguaiaretic acid (NDGA) (15 microliters of 4 mM NDGA/hemisphere, calculated to give an equivalent intracerebral concentration of 120 microM) or the PLA2 inhibitor aristolochic acid (AST) (5 microliters of a 4 mM AST/hemisphere, calculated to give an equivalent intracerebral concentration of approximately 40 microM) were made into the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV), an area that is of crucial importance for memory formation in the chick in this task.
The intermediate and medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) is a memory system in the chick forebrain in which certain learning-related changes occur after imprinting.
It could also be involved in the organization of avoidance behaviour associated with the task, or it could form part of a circuit linking two other forebrain regions previously implicated in one-trial PAL, the intermediate part of the medial hyperstriatum ventrale and the lobus parolfactorius..
The intermediate and medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) in the forebrain of the domestic chick Gallus gallus domesticus has been shown in previous studies to be critically involved in the learning process of imprinting.
It was possible to localize these changes in Na+,K(+)-ATPase activity into forebrain structures contained within the dorsal ventricular ridge comprising the hyperstriatum accessorium (HA), hyperstriatum ventrale (HV), hyperstriatum dorsale (HD), and parts of neostriatum (N).
The anatomical results indicate four circuits by which auditory information may influence the vocal motor system: (1) direct auditory thalamic projections from nucleus dorsomedialis posterior (DMP) to both the neostriatal higher vocal center (HVC) and robust archistriatal nucleus (RA); (2) direct projections from a neostriatal projection field of DMP (i.e., MAN, the magnocellular nucleus of the neostriatum) to HVC and RA; (3) projections from DMP and other 'accessory' auditory thalamic nuclei to the ventral paleostriatum (VP), which in turn projects to MAN and RA; (4) projections to HVC from the lateral hyperstriatum ventrale (HV), which receives input from nucleus basalis (Bas) as well as from the oval nucleus of the HV (HVo), which receives direct input from RA.
The intermediate medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV), part of the avian forebrain, is essential for early learning in the domestic chick.
Termination of thalamic efferents: The thalamic auditory nuclei ovoidalis (Ov) and semilunaris parovoidalis (SPO) project predominantly upon Field L2, and possibly sparsely upon L1, L3 and the overlying hyperstriatum ventrale (HV).
Four-day-old chicks were injected with dynorphin(1-13) into the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale and trained on either a peck avoidance (PA) or an appetitive discrimination (AD) task; 2-day-old chicks were trained on PA.
In situ hybridization reveals that the gamma 4-subunit mRNA is abundant in several brain regions, including the ectostriatum, nucleus rotundus and hyperstriatum ventrale, which are involved in visual processing and learning..
All stations of the ascending pathway displayed high activity levels, including the inferior colliculus, the nucleus ovoidalis of the thalamus, and field L1 to L3 and the hyperstriatum ventrale caudale which correspond to primary and secondary auditory cortex.
This increase occurs in two forebrain areas: the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale and the lobus parolfactorius. Bursting was recorded from the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale of anaesthetized methyl- and water-chicks at eight time-points over the period 1-9 h post-test. These results suggest that the training-induced increase in bursting seen in the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale of methyl-chicks is not a simple, generalized increase with time but rather has a significant temporal aspect.
Unilateral intracranial injection into the left intermediate and medial hyperstriatum ventrale of 100 mumol.liter-1 7-chlorokynurenate, a highly selective antagonist of the glycine site on the N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor, prevents acquisition of memory for one-trial passive avoidance training in day-old chicks, in which the aversive stimulus is an unpleasant tasting substance, methyl anthranilate. Unilateral injections into the left intermediate and medial hyperstriatum ventrale were found to significantly block memory acquisition when birds were tested at all three time points, but unilateral injections in the right intermediate and medial hyperstriatum ventrale had no significant effect.
We investigated the volume of hyperstriatum ventrale, pars caudale, nucleus robustus archistriatalis, and area X of the lobus parolfactorius as defined with the use of a Nissl stain.
Intracerebral injections before training into the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV), an area that is of crucial importance in learning in the chick, produced amnesia in this task when tested at various time points from 30 min to 24 h after training.
Intensely stained neurons and fibers were found in most parts of the telencephalon, in particular in the neostriatum, paleostriatum augmentatum, olfactory tubercle, lobus parolfactorius, hyperstriatum accessorium, and hyperstriatum ventrale.
The archistriatum, the nucleus accumbens, the nucleus of the stria terminalis, the hyperstriatum ventrale and the lateral septum showed moderate to strong staining. We also observed an intensely stained area ventral to the fasciculus prosencephali lateralis and lateral to the tractus septomesencephalicus, a weakly to moderately stained band ventral to the lobus parolfactorius, an intensely stained zone along the lateral ventricle in the hyperstriatum ventrale, and an unstained almond-shaped nucleus in the lateral hyperstriatum ventrale.
There were decreases in binding in the hyperstriatum dorsale of the left hemisphere (14%) and a decrease in binding in the lateral hyperstriatum ventrale of the right hemisphere (14%).
A quantitative ultrastructural study of synapses was carried out in the forebrain IMHV (intermediate and medial hyperstriatum ventrale) of 1-day-old chicks 30 min after training to avoid pecking at a bead coated with methyl anthranilate.
The present study estimates the mean synaptic density (Nv syn) in a region of the chick forebrain known to be involved in memory formation, the intermediate and medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV), 1 and 24 hr following one-trial passive avoidance training.
Previous studies have indicated that glutamate binding levels in the left intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) region of the chick forebrain increase as a correlate of the acquisition of an imprinting memory; however, it has not been determined whether this is due to an increase in the number of glutamate receptors or to increased receptor affinity. We have performed Scatchard displacement binding analyses to examine changes in both the number and affinity of glutamate receptors in the left and right hyperstriatum ventrale (HV) and also the left and right archistriatal/lobus parolfactorius (AS/LPO) areas from imprinted and non-imprinted chicks.
In 2 experiments we explored the effects of lateral versus medial laminar lesions of the hyperstriatum in pigeons (Columba livia); medical lesions were largely confined to the hyperstriatum accessorium, and lateral lesions to the hyperstriatum dorsale and hyperstriatum ventrale.
One region, the intermediate and medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV), is probably a site of long-term memory; the other, the wulst, contains somatic sensory and visual projection areas.
The expression of the gamma protein kinase C isoenzyme (PKC gamma) and of the c-fos immediate early gene protein product Fos in the intermediate and medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) of day-old chicks was determined immunocytochemically.
For the formation of imprinting in birds, protein synthesis is known to be essential in the medial hyperstriatum ventrale (MHV) of the forebrain after presentation of an imprinting stimulus.
Several lines of evidence (biochemical, neuroanatomical, electrophysiological, and behavioural) have indicated a critical role for the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale of the chick forebrain in the acquisition of a passive avoidance response. Previous lesion studies indicated that bilateral or left, but not right, pretraining intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale lesions interfere with the acquisition of this task. We have further analysed this asymmetrical involvement of the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale by use of a monocular learning protocol and intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale lesions (sham, bilateral, or unilateral). As in binocular conditions, bilateral pretraining intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale lesions impair learning in monocularly trained animals. Unilateral lesions to either left or right monocularly trained experimental animals resulted in amnesia when they were made to the right intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale and the chicks were trained/tested with the left eye open. These results indicate that, although right intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale lesions do not result in amnesia in binocular animals, this region is capable of participating in memory acquisition processes. They also suggest a connection between lateralization of intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale function in passive avoidance learning and the behavioural and structural visual asymmetries known to occur in chicks..
Previous research has shown that NIVL receives projections from Field 'L' as well as adjacent portions of the dorsolateral neostriatum intermedium (NIDL) and hyperstriatum ventrale (HV) and, therefore, may be homologous to previously described auditory centers in the dorsal and lateral portions of the auditory neostriatum of songbirds.
The effects of glutamate, N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA), (+)-5-methyl-10,11-dihydro-5H-dibenzo-(a,d)-cyclo-hepten 5,10-imine maleate (MK801), alpha-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazole propionate (AMPA) and quisqualate on the accumulation of inositol phosphates (IP) from the breakdown of phosphoinositides in vitro have been studied in tissue prisms derived from a region of the chick forebrain, the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV).
Responses of units in the auditory forebrain (field L/hyperstriatum ventrale-complex) of awake domestic chicks were studied to frequency-modulated (FM) signals and isointensity tone bursts, presented to the ear contralateral to the recording sites.
This region is the intermediate and medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV).
To understand the circuits that underlie these complex properties, the combination sensitivity of single units in the hyperstriatum ventrale, pars caudale (HVc) of urethane-anesthetized zebra finches was studied.
The region is the intermediate and medial part of the left hyperstriatum ventrale (left IMHV).
Of the regions examined, elevations in the titre of YL1/2 were found in the left intermediate hyperstriatum ventrale 1 h, 6 h and 24 h following training, in the left lobus parolfactorius 1 h following training and in the right lobus parolfactorius 6 h and 24 h following training.
The optomoter environment also produced high activity in the medial hyperstriatum ventrale (MHV), a region that has been implicated in memory formation of imprinting.
Major telencephalic projections of the shell terminated within the ventral paleostriatal complex, "end-zones" of the field L, the caudomedial hyperstriatum ventrale, and regions immediately dorsal and lateral to the auditory neostriatum.
Very high densities of all three ligands were found in the hyperstriatum ventrale; the nucleus geniculatus lateralis, pars ventralis; the griseum tectale; the nucleus dorsolateralis anterior thalami; the nucleus lentiformis mesencephali, pars lateralis and pars medialis; the periventricular organ; and the stratum griseum et fibrosum superficiale, layer f of the optic tectum.
As a first approach to the spatial representation of such sounds in the central auditory system, we have analyzed 2-deoxyglucose (2DG) patterns that were produced by FM stimuli in the tonotopic map of the auditory forebrain area (field L/hyperstriatum ventrale complex) of domestic chicks.
Training chicks on a one-trial passive avoidance task results in memory-dependent synaptic remodeling in the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) and lobus parolfactorius (LPO).
Tissue samples from intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) and lobus parolfactorius (LPO) were isolated at 6 and 24 h after training.
Two discrete areas of the chick brain, the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV) and lobus parolfactorius (LPO), were found to have different functions during the formation of memory for a 1-trial peck-avoidance paradigm.
These immunoreactive cells filled and outlined the boundaries of the hyperstriatum ventrale, pars caudalis, nucleus magnocellularis neostriatalis anterioris (both in the lateral and medial subdivisions), and nucleus robustus archistriatalis.
In an in vitro slice preparation of the chick brain it is possible to record responses to single electrical stimuli from within the intermediate and medial part of the hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV), a region known to be involved in learning.
-
[ View All ]